Open Access
5 September 2018 Atypical neural modulation in the right prefrontal cortex during an inhibitory task with eye gaze in autism spectrum disorder as revealed by functional near-infrared spectroscopy
Takahiro Ikeda, Masahiro Hirai, Takeshi Sakurada, Yukifumi Monden, Tatsuya Tokuda, Masako Nagashima, Hideo Shimoizumi, Ippeita Dan, Takanori Yamagata
Author Affiliations +
Abstract
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by impairment in social communication and the presence of restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests. Executive function impairment is reportedly partially responsible for these symptoms. Executive function includes planning, flexibility, and inhibitory control. Although planning and flexibility in ASD have been consistently reported as atypical, the atypicality of inhibitory control remains controversial. As most previous studies have used nonsocial stimuli to investigate inhibitory control in ASD, the effects of socially relevant information on the inhibitory control system in individuals with ASD remain unclear. Therefore, we developed a go/no-go task with gaze stimuli and measured hemodynamic responses in the right prefrontal cortex (PFC), involved in inhibitory processing in both typically developing (TD) children and children with ASD, using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Direct gaze induced commission errors to similar extents in both groups. Contrary to the behavioral responses, neural activation in the right PFC was modulated by gaze direction only in the TD group. These findings suggest that the gaze-processing mechanisms in the prefrontal region may be affected by atypical gaze processing in other brain regions during an inhibitory control task with socially relevant information in ASD.

1.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by impairments in social interaction and communication accompanied by repetitive stereotyped behaviors and restricted interests.1 In addition, studies have shown atypicality of executive functions (EFs) in ASD.24

EFs are defined as functions regulating various cognitive processes necessary for goal-directed behavior, including planning, flexibility, working memory, attention, and inhibition.3,5 It should be noted that not all domains of EF are impaired in individuals with ASD, but rather selective impairment, such as in planning and flexibility, has been reported. In contrast, most studies have shown that the function of inhibitory control in ASD is largely preserved.3,68

The right prefrontal cortex (PFC) is a candidate brain region for the neural substrate underlying inhibitory control. The potential involvement of this region is supported by several neuropsychological and developmental studies.9,10 Findings in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), who exhibit deficits mainly in inhibitory control, indicate that atypical hemodynamic responses may also underlie the altered inhibitory response in children with ASD.11,12 Most studies have shown a hypoactivation pattern in the right PFC region.10,13 In our previous studies, hemodynamic responses in the right PFC were robustly increased in typically developing (TD) children, but reduced in children with ADHD, during a go/no-go task, as revealed by functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS).1419

Despite the growing evidence of atypical hemodynamic response patterns in the right PFC in children with ADHD during an inhibitory task, it remains unclear whether the neural responses related to inhibitory function are preserved in children with ASD. In a study of inhibitory control in adults with ASD, Schmitz et al.20 found hyperactivation in the bilateral inferior frontal areas during a go/no-go task. In contrast, two other studies have demonstrated that, compared to control groups, adults with ASD exhibit reduced hemodynamic responses in the right inferior frontal gyrus during go/no-go tasks.21,22 Moreover, differences in neural connectivity in the PFC have been reported between individuals with ASD and controls.23,24

Atypical patterns of hemodynamic responses in the PFC region have also been reported in ASD;2024 however, these findings remain controversial. This controversy may be attributed to the different types of cognitive tasks used in functional neuroimaging. These studies have mainly adopted nonsocial artificial stimuli, such as geometric figures, alphabets, and animated characters, to reveal the neural responses in the PFC region.2026 However, because the atypical processing of socially relevant information in individuals with ASD has been established, it is necessary to examine the function of inhibitory control in the context of socially natural experimental conditions. The PFC region is part of the social brain network, which processes socially relevant stimuli and tasks. For example, gaze direction could modulate dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) activation when participants viewed faces with averted and direct gazes.27 Therefore, it is likely that socially relevant information can affect hemodynamic responses in the PFC during social tasks.

In addition, a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study revealed differential cortical responses in the DLPFC induced by gaze direction in the TD and ASD groups. In this study, the participants were instructed to watch an animation of a walking man with his gaze directed toward or averted from them. The authors found that the patterns of neural responses in the DLPFC were enhanced in the TD group when the participants observed an averted gaze; however, the opposite pattern was observed in the ASD group.28

Therefore, in the present study, we aimed to examine the behavioral and hemodynamic correlations between inhibitory control and socially relevant visual stimuli in children with and without ASD. Because we had observed hypoactivation in the right PFC region in children with ADHD using fNIRS,1419 we focused on the specific activation pattern in the right PFC during a go/no-go task with direct and averted gazes. We reasoned that if the function of inhibitory control was modulated atypically by socially relevant information in children with ASD, both their behavioral and hemodynamic responses in the right PFC would also be atypical compared to those in the TD group.

2.

Methods

2.1.

Participants

Twenty-two individuals with ASD (17 boys, mean age: 12.9 years, S.D.: 2.8 years) were recruited from Jichi Medical University (Shimotsuke, Tochigi, Japan) and the International University of Health and Welfare (Otawara, Tochigi, Japan). As a control group, 24 healthy TD children (12 boys, mean age: 13.6 years, S.D.: 2.2 years) participated in the study. The TD children were recruited from elementary, junior high, and high schools located near Jichi Medical University and the International University of Health and Welfare, and had no history of psychiatric or neurological problems. There was a marginally significant sex difference between the groups (p=0.07, Fisher’s exact test). The ages of the two groups were not significantly different [t(44)=0.96, p=0.34]. The intelligence quotient (IQ) scores of the participants were tested with the Japanese version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children III and IV (WISC-III and WISC-IV) (Japanese WISC-III Publication Committee, 1998; Japanese WISC-IV Publication Committee, 2010). The two editions contain the same material with minor differences, have continuity in their structures, and have shown strong correlations with full-scale IQ tests (r=0.89).29 The IQ scores of the participants with ASD (mean IQ: 97.9, S.D.: 14.6, range: 71 to 121) were significantly lower [t(44)=2.32, p=0.03] than those of the TD controls (mean IQ: 106.5, S.D.: 10.4, range: 91 to 126) (Table 1). All participants and their parents provided written informed consent before the experiment, which was conducted in conformity with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Jichi Medical University and the International University of Health and Welfare.

Table 1

Demographic and clinical profiles for ASD and TD subjects.

GroupN (M/F)Chronological ageIQAQPARS
Mean (years)±SDMean±SD (range)Mean±SD (range)Mean±SD (range)
Range (years; months)
ASD22 (17/5)12.9±2.8 (8;4 to 17;8)97.9±14.6 (71 to 121)24.9±13.5 (10 to 43)28.3±11.4 (11 to 54)
TD24 (12/12)13.6±2.20 (9;0 to 17;0)106.5±10.4 (91 to 126)10.6±5.5 (2 to 19)1.75±2.7 (0 to 11)
Note: ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TD, typical development; M, male; F, female; IQ, intelligence quotient; AQ, autism-spectrum quotient; PARS, Pervasive Developmental Disorders Autism Society Japan Rating Scale.

2.2.

Psychiatric Assessment

The diagnosis of the participants with ASD was established by trained pediatric neurologists (T.I., Y.M., M.N., H.S., and H.W.) based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) criteria.1 The diagnosis was further confirmed by two questionnaires: the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ)30 test and the Pervasive Developmental Disorders Autism Society Japan Rating Scale (PARS).31 The AQ is an instrument used for evaluating autistic traits, with a scoring range from 0 to 50, that has been validated for the Japanese population.32 In this experiment, we used the AQ children version, and the mean score of the participants with ASD was 24.9 (S.D.: 13.5, range: 10 to 43), while that of the TD controls was 10.6 (S.D.: 5.5, range: 2 to 19). The PARS is a semistructured interview in Japanese assessing the severity of autistic symptoms, and its score correlates with that of the Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised (r=0.41).31 It includes 57 items describing autistic characteristics, with 34 of them representing behaviors observed during infancy, 33 during childhood, and 33 during adolescence and adulthood. In this study, 33 of the items on childhood or adolescence/adulthood behaviors were adopted. The mean PARS score of the participants with ASD was 28.3 (S.D.: 11.4, range: 11 to 54), and that of the TD controls was 1.8 (S.D.: 2.7, range: 0 to 11).

2.3.

Stimuli and Apparatus

In the present study, we developed a new inhibitory task that combines the go/no-go task with socially relevant cues. In this task, a green or red dot was superimposed between the eyebrows of a female or male face, with a direct or averted gaze, generated from FaceGen®. The participants were seated facing a screen presenting these stimuli and asked to press a button only when a green (= go task), but not red (= no-go task), dot appeared. We used E-Prime® (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania) to create and present the stimuli and collect the behavioral responses.

2.4.

Experimental Procedure

In this experiment, a block contained a go trial (in which only faces with closed eyes and a green dot between the eyebrows were shown) and a go/no-go trial (in which faces with either direct or averted gazes, and with either a green or red dot between the eyebrows, were shown). The participants underwent two sessions consisting of six blocks each, including three blocks for the direct gaze condition and three blocks for the averted gaze condition. Each trial in a block lasted for 24 s preceded by a 3-s instruction, “press the button for the green dot” or “do not press the button for the red dot.” The stimuli were displayed on the screen once per second. Therefore, in total, 24 tasks were displayed in a go/no-go trial (Fig. 1). The go/no-go trials included 50% of no-go tasks. The go/no-go ratio was determined based on previous behavioral and neuroimaging studies.6,1419,3336 To press the button, the participants had to focus their attention on the dots between the eyebrows. This ensured that the participants always observed the direction of the gaze during a trial. Because we displayed the face stimuli with closed eyes in the baseline period (the go trial), the neural responses specific for gaze direction could be isolated in the subsequent go/no-go trial. Gaze direction has been suggested to facilitate social cognitive function37,38 and induce neural responses in the PFC.39,40 Therefore, we sought to identify the effect of gaze stimuli on the inhibitory function in the PFC region.

Fig. 1

Experimental design and stimuli. (a) Direct or averted gazes with a green or red dot between the eyes were displayed as socially relevant visual stimuli. (b) Each block (direct or averted gaze condition) contained one go trial and one go/no-go trial. The duration of each trial was 24 s.

NPH_5_3_035008_f001.png

2.5.

fNIRS Measurements

fNIRS is a noninvasive neuroimaging tool for continuous measurement of cerebral cortical hemodynamics. Near-infrared light is irradiated at two wavelengths (695 and 830 nm), and signals are measured reflecting changes in the concentrations of oxygenated (oxy-Hb) and deoxygenated (deoxy-Hb) hemoglobin in the brain tissue at 25 to 30 mm from the surface, based on the modified Beer–Lambert Law.41 Since the oxy-Hb concentration increases and the deoxy-Hb concentration decreases in activated brain regions because of neurovascular coupling, changes in these parameters signify focal hemodynamic responses. Accordingly, we calculated signals reflecting oxy-Hb and deoxy-Hb concentration changes, expressed in units of millimolar·millimeter (mM·mm).42 We placed 44 channels over the bilateral PFC in the parietal scalp area to measure the oxy-Hb and deoxy-Hb signals in the frontal lobe, related to inhibitory control, with an ETG-4000® multichannel fNIRS system (Hitachi Medical Corporation, Kashiwa, Japan)1419 (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2

Spatial profiles of fNIRS channels. (a) The probe and channel locations on the scalp are shown in both left- and right-side views. The probes covered the bilateral frontal and temporal regions. The blue circles indicate detectors emitting infrared radiation, and the red circles indicate illuminators that receive reflected light. The numbers in the white squares between the blue and red circles indicate channel numbers. (b) Probabilistically estimated fNIRS channel locations on the brain surface (centers of blue circles) for all participants and their spatial variability (SD, radii of the blue circles) associated with the estimation are depicted in a Montreal Neurological Institute space.

NPH_5_3_035008_f002.png

For all participants, the position of each channel was obtained using a three-dimensional-digitizer (Patriot Digitizer®, Polhemus) after the fNIRS measurement, and the data were matched to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain template form. This procedure is an established method of probabilistic registration and reveals the anatomical position of channels on the brain surface in both adults and children.4346

2.6.

Data Analysis

As we found statistically significant differences in mean IQ scores between the ASD and TD groups, which could potentially affect results, we initially attempted to perform an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) of both the behavioral performance and fNIRS data with the IQ score as a covariate. However, because no significant regression was found, ANCOVA was not appropriate to analyze the data. In addition, no significant correlations were observed in both groups between the IQ scores and behavioral measures (ASD: rs<0.18; TD: rs<0.17) or between the IQ scores and differential neural responses (ASD: r=0.10; and TD: r=0.02). We therefore assessed performance using mixed-design repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA).

2.6.1.

Behavioral performance

We analyzed reaction time and both commission and omission errors as measures of behavioral performance. For statistical analysis, we performed a two-way mixed-design repeated-measures (ANOVA) with group (TD versus ASD) and gaze direction (direct versus averted) as between- and within-subjects factors, respectively.

2.6.2.

fNIRS data

We used the oxy-Hb signal because of its higher sensitivity to changes in cerebral blood flow,4749 signal-to-noise ratio,49 and retest reliability50 compared to those of the deoxy-Hb and total-Hb signals. The raw oxy-Hb concentration data were bandpass-filtered from 0.01 to 0.5 Hz to remove baseline drift and heartbeat pulsations, similar to previous studies.1419 Blocks with marked motion-related artifacts were removed, and we analyzed the data of the participants for whom more than four out of the six blocks remained in each direct or averted gaze condition. We averaged the waveforms of the oxy-Hb concentration from 4 to 27 s after the go/no-go block onset as the target period. This average was compared to the average of the baseline period from 10 to 0 s before the go block onset, as in the previous studies.1419

Because we used experimental procedures for measuring neural activity related to a go/no-go task identical to those used in our previous studies,1417 we selected and analyzed channel 32 (Ch32) as a region of interest (ROI). This channel was located in the region of the right middle frontal gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus (Table 2), where obvious hemodynamic responses related to inhibitory control have been observed in TD individuals.5153 Moreover, hemodynamic responses within this channel are influenced by social stimuli such as gaze direction.27,28,37,38,54,55 We averaged the waveforms of the oxy-Hb concentration and evaluated the hemodynamic responses during the task as the difference in averaged z-transformed oxy-Hb concentration for the baseline period (go task with closed eyes) and target period (go/no-go task with direct or averted gaze), as in the previous studies.56,57 The raw data were transformed into z-scores based on the baseline period from 0 to 10 s after the go block onset, and thus could be compared as normalized data.

Table 2

Spatial profiles of the target channels.

MNI coordinates
Chx, y, z (SD)MacroanatomyProbability (%)Brodmann areaProbability (%)
3242.3, 53.3, 25.3 (13.6)R middle frontal gyrus83.146Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex76.3
R inferior frontal gyrus16.945pars triangularis Broca’s area20.8
10Frontopolar area0.0
Note: MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute; R, right.

A two-way ANOVA was conducted for the statistical analysis of oxy-Hb signals at Ch32. Group was used as a between-subjects factor (TD versus ASD), and gaze direction (direct versus averted) as a within-subjects factor.

2.7.

Correlation Analysis

To evaluate the relationships between individual differences in hemodynamic responses and autistic traits, we examined the correlation between the differential task-related oxy-Hb signal, which is the averaged oxy-Hb signal in the direct gaze condition subtracted from that in the averted gaze condition, and the AQ score with Pearson’s correlation analysis.

3.

Results

3.1.

Behavioral Performance

For the reaction times (Table 3), we did not find any significant main effects (Fs<3.18, all ps>0.08). The main effect of gaze direction on commission errors was significant [F(1,44)=5.03, p<0.05, ηp2=0.10]. This finding indicates that the commission error rate in the direct gaze condition was significantly higher than that in the averted gaze condition in both groups. However, the main effect of group and the interaction of group and gaze direction were not significant (Fs<1.86, all ps>0.17). No significant effects were observed in the analysis of on omission errors (Fs<3.41, all ps>0.07).

Table 3

Behavioral data for ASD and TD subjects.

(a) Means and standard deviations of reaction times and error rates
GazeASD (n=22)TD (n=24)
DirectAvertedDirectAverted
Reaction time (ms) (SD)414.5 (53.1)414.2 (56.0)409.2 (27.1)416.9 (40.8)
Omission errors (%) (SD)1.26 (2.48)0.89 (1.48)0.58 (0.89)0.29 (0.90)
Commission errors (%) (SD)4.61 (4.63)3.28 (4.48)2.84 (3.07)2.26 (2.15)
(b) Two-way ANOVA for gaze direction and group
SourcedfFp
Reaction timeGaze direction (direct versus averted)1, 442.6760.109
Group (ASD versus TD)1, 440.0080.928
Omission errorGaze direction (direct versus averted)1, 443.4050.072
Group (ASD versus TD)1, 442.1930.146
Commission errorGaze direction (direct versus averted)1, 445.0290.030*
Group (ASD versus TD)1, 441.8510.180
Note: ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TD, typical development; Df, degrees of freedom; F, F value; p, p value.

*p<0.05.

3.2.

fNIRS Analyses

We found a significant two-way interaction between group and gaze direction for the z-transformed mean oxy-Hb signals [F(1,44)=5.23, p<0.05, ηp2=0.11]. To explore the nature of this interaction, tests of simple main effects were performed. The main effect of gaze direction was significant in the TD group [F(1,44)=7.02, p<0.05, ηp2=0.14], but not in the ASD group [F(1,44)=0.34, p=0.56, ηp2=0.01] (Fig. 3). This result indicates that the oxy-Hb signal in the averted gaze condition was significantly higher than that in the direct gaze condition in the TD group. However, the main effect of group within each gaze direction was not significant [Fs<2.50, all ps>0.11].

Fig. 3

(a) Waveforms of the oxy-hemoglobin (oxy-Hb) signal and (b) comparison of the average oxy-Hb levels in Ch32 between TD children and children with ASD. (a) The waveforms indicate averaged oxy-Hb signals in the direct gaze condition (blue line) and averted gaze condition (red line) for Ch32 located on the right PFC in the TD and ASD groups. (b) Mean oxy-Hb levels from 4 to 24 s for each channel. The blue bars indicate the direct gaze condition, and the red bars indicate the averted gaze condition. The error bars indicate the standard error of the mean.

NPH_5_3_035008_f003.png

3.3.

Oxy-Hb Signal Data

The time courses of the grand-averaged oxy-Hb signal in Ch32 for the TD and ASD groups are shown in Fig. 3(a). The averaged signals were elevated at the onset of the task in both gaze conditions in both groups; however, they decreased substantially only in the direct gaze condition in the TD group. Therefore, the averaged oxy-Hb signals for the target period had opposite patterns in the two gaze directions in the TD group [Fig 3(b)].

3.4.

Analysis of Correlations Between Behavioral/Neural Performance and Scores

We further explored the relationship between the differential task-related oxy-Hb signals (direct versus averted) during the task and behavioral scores. We found a significant negative correlation between the AQ scores and the differential oxy-Hb signals at Ch32 in the ASD group (r=0.45, p=0.04), but not in the TD group (r=0.11, p=0.60) (Fig. 4). A marginally significant difference was identified between these two correlation coefficients (z=1.88, p=0.06).

Fig. 4

Correlation between oxy-Hb signal in Ch32 (direct minus averted) and AQ score. The scatterplots illustrate how AQ scores were associated with mean differential oxy-Hb signal responses (averaged oxy-Hb signal in the direct gaze condition minus that in the averted gaze condition). The green circles indicate TD participants, and the yellow rhombs indicate participants with ASD. A significant correlation between the oxy-Hb levels and AQ scores was observed only in the ASD group.

NPH_5_3_035008_f004.png

4.

Discussion

In the present study, direct gaze had a significant overall impact on the behavioral data in both groups. There were no significant differences between the ASD and TD groups in reaction time or error rates. The frequency of commission errors in the direct gaze condition was significantly higher than that in the averted gaze condition in both groups. Contrary to the behavioral data, averted gaze had a significant impact on the oxy-Hb responses in the right PFC region in the TD group, but not in the ASD group. The mean oxy-Hb levels were significantly correlated with the AQ scores in the ASD group. These results indicate that gaze direction during the inhibitory task affected behavioral performance in both groups, whereas inhibition-related neural responses in the right PFC region were modulated by gaze direction only in TD individuals. We initially predicted that direct gaze would have a differential impact on both behavioral and neural responses. Our prediction was partly borne out by the mean oxy-Hb response levels, but not by the behavioral measures.

Consistent with previous findings of the significant effect of direct gaze in TD children and adults,5863 the commission error rate in the direct gaze condition was significantly higher than that in the averted gaze condition in the TD group. However, we found a similar effect in the ASD group. Most previous studies on gaze processing in ASD have reported atypical eye-gaze processing in infancy and childhood.37,6467 Furthermore, gaze stimuli have a great impact on other cognitive functions such as memory and face recognition.38,55 These studies have shown that cognitive abilities in TD children are facilitated by direct gaze; however, this is not the case for children with ASD.

Our behavioral findings in the ASD group are inconsistent with the previous results, which may be explained by our task design and instructions. Kikuchi et al.68 reported that atypical disengagement from faces was diminished in children with ASD when they were instructed to fixate on the eye region. In line with these findings, when individuals with ASD were instructed to fixate on the eyes, it also altered the hemodynamic responses in the fusiform region and amygdala, which are involved in face processing.62,69 These results imply that the explicit instruction of fixating on the eye region in our experiment may have altered behavioral responses in children with ASD, which in turn may have led to their behavioral performance being comparable to that of the control group.

We found that commission errors in the direct gaze condition were significantly increased compared to those in the averted gaze condition. Previous studies have demonstrated that perceived social stimuli, such as direct gaze, influence various aspects of cognitive performance. For example, when participants fixate on direct-gaze faces, but not on averted-gaze faces, the reaction time is prolonged to detect peripheral targets.61,70,71 Moreover, a go/no-go task with emotional faces induced a higher rate of commission errors compared to that in a task with nonsocial stimuli.72 These findings suggest that social stimuli can capture visual attention. Another possibility is that the significantly increased error rate may be attributable to an elevated arousal level.73 Therefore, inhibitory responses may be less functional in the direct gaze condition compared to the averted gaze condition.

In contrast with the behavioral findings, we found differences in the mean oxy-Hb signals at Ch32 depending on the displayed direction of gaze. Ch32 was located in the ROI encompassing the right middle and inferior frontal gyri (Table 2). Although recent neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that the right inferior frontal cortex plays an important role in inhibition control,10,74 meta-analyses have shown that the right-lateral prefrontal cortex, including the middle and inferior frontal gyri, is associated with performance in go/no-go tasks.53,75 Moreover, other studies have found developmental changes in the neural activation pattern during inhibitory tasks: from childhood to adulthood, the pattern shifts from the middle to the inferior frontal gyrus region.76,77 Therefore, given that the participants in the present study were mainly children and adolescents, we consider our ROI selection reasonable. In addition, in our previous fNIRS studies,1419 we selected Ch32 as an ROI located in the inferior frontal gyrus (22% to 33%) and middle frontal gyrus (63% to 78%), and found robust neural responses during an inhibition task. The mean oxy-Hb signals during the task were significantly more enhanced in the averted gaze condition than in the direct gaze condition in the TD group, but not in the ASD group. Therefore, the right PFC region may be sensitive to social stimuli as part of the social brain network.78,79 Supporting this possibility, several neuroimaging studies on gaze perception have reported that hemodynamic responses in the DLPFC region are also modulated by the perception of gaze direction.27,28,80 Moreover, the neural responses to gaze direction were modulated differently in the TD and ASD groups.28,80

As we did not find any group differences in behavioral responses, we speculate that differential neural mechanisms may be involved in the prefrontal regions. Several studies have reported that the behavioral performance of ASD individuals is comparable to that of typical controls despite differences in neural activity related to the cognitive task.8184 In these studies, differential neural responses were observed in the ASD and TD groups during a selective attention task and in socially relevant tasks. In line with the results of these studies, it is likely that atypical gaze information processing (possibly in other brain regions such as the posterior superior temporal sulcus) may have affected neural activity in the PFC region in the ASD group in the present study. Consequently, we observed different neural responses to gaze direction in TD and ASD subjects.

We found a significant positive correlation between the mean oxy-Hb levels and the total AQ scores only in the ASD group. Concordant with these findings, some studies have shown that neural activity and behavioral performance are correlated with autistic traits in ASD.40,8587 Two of these studies further analyzed the correlation with autistic traits in a TD group. One reported no correlation,85 and the other reported an inverse correlation.86 The variable results may be attributable to ASD encompassing a wide continuum of clinical features; however, the mechanism of the differences in correlation is unclear. Further studies are needed to explain the discrepancy found in the present study.

Several limitations should be acknowledged. First, the overall sample size of the present study was small, and the numbers of ASD and TD individuals were different, resulting in a significant, if only marginally, sex difference between the two groups. To increase accuracy, future studies with increased sample sizes are necessary. Second, our study did not examine neural activity in brain regions other than the right PFC. We did not elucidate the involvement of other social brain network regions, most importantly the posterior part of the superior temporal sulcus, which has been implicated in gaze processing by numerous studies.88 In addition, recent studies have reported that the PFC is not functionally unique in its inhibitory control.89,90 Further study is needed to investigate how other brain regions related to inhibition and the processing of socially related information may be involved in the task used in the present study. Third, we adopted a block design with 50% of go/no-go trials, similar to other studies using 50% of go/no-go trials6,3336 and a block design.33,36,9194 Our previous studies that employed 50% of go/no-go trials revealed robust neural responses in the right PFC.1416 However, several other studies have shown that a lower fraction of no-go trials, such as 20%, requires an inhibition function called a prepotent response, which in turn induces a stronger overall inhibitory response.89,95 Furthermore, although a block design can have statistical power superior to an event-related design, it cannot easily distinguish responses to specific behavioral outcomes within a block (e.g., correct and incorrect tasks). Therefore, further studies should adopt a task design that induces prepotent responses and an event-related overall design to more clearly reveal the inhibitory responses.

In conclusion, our results suggest that gaze direction has a significant impact on inhibitory control in both behavioral and hemodynamic responses that differs between ASD and TD individuals. Whereas the behavioral measures showed similar patterns in the ASD and TD groups, hemodynamic responses in the right PFC region were modulated differentially by gaze direction in the two groups. These results may reflect the atypical gaze processing mechanisms in the prefrontal regions of ASD individuals during an inhibitory task.

Disclosures

All authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all the participants, as well as their caregivers, for their participation. We wish to thank H. Watanabe for recruiting participants and T. Mizutani, H. Masuda, M. Ichikawa, and M. Ishijima for assistance with data collection. This work was supported by a Jichi Medical University Young Investigator Award to T.I., and a grant-in-aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (15H01585; Constructive Developmental Science) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology to M.H.

References

1. 

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5, American Psychiatric Publishing, Washington, D.C. (2013). Google Scholar

2. 

M. C. Lai, M. V. Lombardo and S. Baron-Cohen, “Autism,” Lancet, 383 (9920), 896 –910 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61539-1 JPSAESLANCAO 0734-28290140-6736 Google Scholar

3. 

E. L. Hill, “Executive dysfunction in autism,” Trends Cognit. Sci., 8 (1), 26 –32 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003 Google Scholar

4. 

N. Russo et al., “Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: implications for cognitive neuroscience,” Brain Cognit., 65 (1), 77 –86 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.007 Google Scholar

5. 

D. T. Stuss and M. P. Alexander, “Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view,” Psychol. Res., 63 (3-4), 289 –298 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/s004269900007 Google Scholar

6. 

S. Ozonoff et al., “Executive function abilities in autism and Tourette syndrome: an information processing approach,” J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry, 35 (6), 1015 –1032 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.1994.35.issue-6 JPPDAI 0021-9630 Google Scholar

7. 

S. Ozonoff and D. L. Strayer, “Inhibitory function in nonretarded children with autism,” J. Autism. Dev. Disord., 27 (1), 59 –77 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025821222046 JADDDQ Google Scholar

8. 

F. Craig et al., “A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder,” Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat., 12 1191 –1202 (2016). https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S104620 Google Scholar

9. 

A. R. Aron et al., “Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans,” Nat. Neurosci., 6 (2), 115 –116 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1003 NANEFN 1097-6256 Google Scholar

10. 

A. R. Aron, T. W. Robbins and R. A. Poldrack, “Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex,” Trends Cognit. Sci., 8 (4), 170 –177 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010 Google Scholar

11. 

R. Schachar et al., “Deficient inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder,” J. Abnorm. Child Psychol., 23 (4), 411 –437 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01447206 JABCAA 0091-0627 Google Scholar

12. 

R. A. Barkley, “Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD,” Psychol. Bull., 121 (1), 65 –94 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65 PSBUAI 0033-2909 Google Scholar

13. 

K. Rubia et al., “Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD,” Am. J. Psychiatry, 162 (6), 1067 –1075 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1067 Google Scholar

14. 

Y. Monden et al., “Clinically-oriented monitoring of acute effects of methylphenidate on cerebral hemodynamics in ADHD children using fNIRS,” Clin. Neurophysiol., 123 (6), 1147 –1157 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2011.10.006 CNEUFU 1388-2457 Google Scholar

15. 

Y. Monden et al., “Right prefrontal activation as a neuro-functional biomarker for monitoring acute effects of methylphenidate in ADHD children: an fNIRS study,” Neuroimage Clin., 1 (1), 131 –140 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.001 Google Scholar

16. 

Y. Monden et al., “Individual classification of ADHD children by right prefrontal hemodynamic responses during a go/no-go task as assessed by fNIRS,” Neuroimage Clin., 9 1 –12 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2015.06.011 Google Scholar

17. 

M. Nagashima et al., “Acute neuropharmacological effects of atomoxetine on inhibitory control in ADHD children: a fNIRS study,” Neuroimage Clin., 6 192 –201 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2014.09.001 Google Scholar

18. 

M. Nagashima et al., “Neuropharmacological effect of methylphenidate on attention network in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during oddball paradigms as assessed using functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” Neurophotonics, 1 (1), 015001 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.1.1.015001 Google Scholar

19. 

M. Nagashima et al., “Neuropharmacological effect of atomoxetine on attention network in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during oddball paradigms as assessed using functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” Neurophotonics, 1 (2), 025007 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.1.2.025007 Google Scholar

20. 

N. Schmitz et al., “Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders,” Biol. Psychiatry, 59 (1), 7 –16 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.007 BIPCBF 0006-3223 Google Scholar

21. 

R. K. Kana et al., “Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks,” Biol. Psychiatry, 62 (3), 198 –206 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.004 BIPCBF 0006-3223 Google Scholar

22. 

E. Daly et al., “Response inhibition and serotonin in autism: a functional MRI study using acute tryptophan depletion,” Brain, 137 (Pt 9), 2600 –2610 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awu178 BRAIAK 0006-8950 Google Scholar

23. 

P. S. Lee et al., “Functional connectivity of the inferior frontal cortex changes with age in children with autism spectrum disorders: a fcMRI study of response inhibition,” Cereb. Cortex, 19 (8), 1787 –1794 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn209 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

24. 

S. Ambrosino et al., “Functional connectivity during cognitive control in children with autism spectrum disorder: an independent component analysis,” J. Neural Transm., 121 (9), 1145 –1155 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-014-1237-8 Google Scholar

25. 

A. S. Vara et al., “Is inhibitory control a ‘no-go’ in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder?,” Mol. Autism, 5 (1), 6 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-5-6 Google Scholar

26. 

T. Xiao et al., “Response inhibition impairment in high functioning autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence from near-infrared spectroscopy data,” PLoS One, 7 (10), e46569 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046569 POLNCL 1932-6203 Google Scholar

27. 

J. Jiang et al., “Neural mechanisms of eye contact when listening to another person talking,” Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci., 12 (2), 319 –328 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw127 Google Scholar

28. 

N. B. Pitskel et al., “Brain mechanisms for processing direct and averted gaze in individuals with autism,” J. Autism Dev. Disord., 41 (12), 1686 –1693 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1197-x Google Scholar

29. 

A. S. Kaufman et al., “Test review: Wechsler intelligence scale for children, (WISC-IV),” J. Psychoeduc. Assess., 24 (3), 278 –295 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282906288389 Google Scholar

30. 

S. Baron-Cohen et al., “The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians,” J. Autism Dev. Disord., 31 (1), 5 –17 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005653411471 Google Scholar

31. 

H. Ito et al., “Validation of an interview-based rating scale developed in Japan for pervasive developmental disorders,” Res. Autism Spectr. Disord., 6 (4), 1265 –1272 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.04.002 Google Scholar

32. 

A. Wakabayashi et al., “The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) in Japan: a cross-cultural comparison,” J. Autism Dev. Disord., 36 (2), 263 –270 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0061-2 Google Scholar

33. 

C. J. Vaidya et al., “Selective effects of methylphenidate in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a functional magnetic resonance study,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 95 (24), 14494 –14499 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.24.14494 Google Scholar

34. 

V. Menon et al., “Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 12 (3), 131 –143 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1002/(ISSN)1097-0193 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

35. 

P. F. Liddle, K. A. Kiehl and A. M. Smith, “Event-related fMRI study of response inhibition,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 12 (2), 100 –109 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1002/(ISSN)1097-0193 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

36. 

W. Dillo et al., “Neuronal correlates of ADHD in adults with evidence for compensation strategies: a functional MRI study with a Go/No-Go paradigm,” Ger. Med. Sci., 8 Doc09 (2010). https://doi.org/10.3205/000098 Google Scholar

37. 

A. Senju et al., “Eye contact does not facilitate detection in children with autism,” Cognition, 89 (1), B43 –B51 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00081-7 CGTNAU 0010-0277 Google Scholar

38. 

E. Pellicano and C. N. Macrae, “Mutual eye gaze facilitates person categorization for typically developing children, but not for children with autism,” Psychon. Bull. Rev., 16 (6), 1094 –1099 (2009). https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.6.1094 PBUREN 1069-9384 Google Scholar

39. 

A. J. Calder et al., “Reading the mind from eye gaze,” Neuropsychologia, 40 (8), 1129 –1138 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00008-8 NUPSA6 0028-3932 Google Scholar

40. 

K. A. Pelphrey, J. P. Morris and G. McCarthy, “Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism,” Brain, 128 (5), 1038 –1048 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh404 BRAIAK 0006-8950 Google Scholar

41. 

M. Cope et al., “Methods of quantitating cerebral near infrared spectroscopy data,” Adv. Exp. Med. Biol., 222 183 –189 (1988). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-9510-6 AEMBAP 0065-2598 Google Scholar

42. 

A. Maki et al., “Spatial and temporal analysis of human motor activity using noninvasive NIR topography,” Med. Phys., 22 (12), 1997 –2005 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1118/1.597496 MPHYA6 0094-2405 Google Scholar

43. 

M. Okamoto and I. Dan, “Automated cortical projection of head-surface locations for transcranial functional brain mapping,” Neuroimage, 26 (1), 18 –28 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.018 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

44. 

A. K. Singh et al., “Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI,” Neuroimage, 27 (4), 842 –851 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.019 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

45. 

D. Tsuzuki et al., “Virtual spatial registration of stand-alone fNIRS data to MNI space,” Neuroimage, 34 (4), 1506 –1518 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.043 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

46. 

D. Tsuzuki et al., “Stable and convenient spatial registration of stand-alone NIRS data through anchor-based probabilistic registration,” Neurosci. Res., 72 (2), 163 –171 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2011.10.008 Google Scholar

47. 

Y. Hoshi, “Functional near-infrared optical imaging: utility and limitations in human brain mapping,” Psychophysiology, 40 (4), 511 –520 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.2003.40.issue-4 PSPHAF 0048-5772 Google Scholar

48. 

Y. Hoshi, N. Kobayashi and M. Tamura, “Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model,” J. Appl. Physiol., 90 (5), 1657 –1662 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.2001.90.5.1657 Google Scholar

49. 

G. Strangman et al., “A quantitative comparison of simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS recordings during functional brain activation,” Neuroimage, 17 (2), 719 –731 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1227 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

50. 

M. Plichta et al., “Event-related functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS): are the measurements reliable?,” Neuroimage, 31 (1), 116 –124 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.008 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

51. 

K. Rubia et al., “Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection,” Neuroimage, 20 (1), 351 –358 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00275-1 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

52. 

K. Rubia et al., “Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different versions of go/no-go and stop tasks,” Neuroimage, 13 (2), 250 –261 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0685 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

53. 

D. J. Simmonds, J. J. Pekar and S. H. Mostofsky, “Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent,” Neuropsychologia, 46 (1), 224 –232 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.015 NUPSA6 0028-3932 Google Scholar

54. 

A. Senju and M. H. Johnson, “Atypical eye contact in autism: models, mechanisms and development,” Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 33 (8), 1204 –1214 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.06.001 NBREDE 0149-7634 Google Scholar

55. 

T. Falck-Ytter, C. Carlstrom and M. Johansson, “Eye contact modulates cognitive processing differently in children with autism,” Child Dev., 86 (1), 37 –47 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.2015.86.issue-1 CHDEAW 0009-3920 Google Scholar

56. 

G. Matsuda, K. Hiraki, “Prefrontal cortex deactivation during video game play,” Gaming, Simulations, and Society, 101 –109 Springer, Tokyo (2005). Google Scholar

57. 

G. Matsuda and K. Hiraki, “Sustained decrease in oxygenated hemoglobin during video games in the dorsal prefrontal cortex: a NIRS study of children,” Neuroimage, 29 (3), 706 –711 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.019 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

58. 

H. Akechi et al., “Absence of preferential unconscious processing of eye contact in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder,” Autism Res., 7 (5), 590 –597 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1397 Google Scholar

59. 

M. von Grunau and C. Anston, “The detection of gaze direction: a stare-in-the-crowd effect,” Perception, 24 (11), 1297 –1313 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1068/p241297 PCTNBA 0301-0066 Google Scholar

60. 

T. Yokoyama et al., “Perception of direct gaze does not require focus of attention,” Sci. Rep., 4 (3858), 1 –7 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/srep03858 SRCEC3 2045-2322 Google Scholar

61. 

A. Senju and T. Hasegawa, “Direct gaze captures visuospatial attention,” Vis. Cognit., 12 (1), 127 –144 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280444000157 1350-6285 Google Scholar

62. 

K. M. Dalton et al., “Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism,” Nat. Neurosci., 8 (4), 519 –526 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1421 NANEFN 1097-6256 Google Scholar

63. 

A. Kylliainen and J. K. Hietanen, “Skin conductance responses to another person’s gaze in children with autism,” J. Autism Dev. Disord., 36 (4), 517 –525 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0091-4 Google Scholar

64. 

S. Baron-Cohen et al., “Psychological markers in the detection of autism in infancy in a large population,” Br. J. Psychiatry, 168 (2), 158 –163 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.168.2.158 BJPYAJ 0007-1250 Google Scholar

65. 

S. Baron-Cohen et al., “Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome,” J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry, 38 (7), 813 –822 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.1997.38.issue-7 JPPDAI 0021-9630 Google Scholar

66. 

T. Charman et al., “Infants with autism: an investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation,” Dev. Psychol., 33 (5), 781 –789 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.5.781 DEVPA9 0012-1649 Google Scholar

67. 

G. Dawson et al., “Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development,” Child Dev., 73 (3), 700 –717 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.2002.73.issue-3 CHDEAW 0009-3920 Google Scholar

68. 

Y. Kikuchi et al., “Atypical disengagement from faces and its modulation by the control of eye fixation in children with autism spectrum disorder,” J. Autism Dev. Disord., 41 (5), 629 –645 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1082-z Google Scholar

69. 

N. Tottenham et al., “Elevated amygdala response to faces and gaze aversion in autism spectrum disorder,” Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci., 9 (1), 106 –117 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst050 Google Scholar

70. 

B. M. Hood et al., “Eye remember you: the effects of gaze direction on face recognition in children and adults,” Dev. Sci., 6 (1), 67 –71 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.2003.6.issue-1 Google Scholar

71. 

A. Senju et al., “Is anyone looking at me? Direct gaze detection in children with and without autism,” Brain Cognit., 67 (2), 127 –139 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.12.001 Google Scholar

72. 

K. P. Schulz et al., “Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog,” Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol., 22 (2), 151 –160 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.001 Google Scholar

73. 

T. M. Helminen, S. M. Kaasinen and J. K. Hietanen, “Eye contact and arousal: the effects of stimulus duration,” Biol. Psychol., 88 (1), 124 –130 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.07.002 Google Scholar

74. 

A. Sebastian et al., “Dissociable attentional and inhibitory networks of dorsal and ventral areas of the right inferior frontal cortex: a combined task-specific and coordinate-based meta-analytic fMRI study,” Brain Struct. Funct., 221 (3), 1635 –1651 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-015-0994-y Google Scholar

75. 

B. J. Levy and A. D. Wagner, “Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and action updating,” Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1224 40 –62 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05958.x ANYAA9 0077-8923 Google Scholar

76. 

K. Rubia et al., “Progressive increase of frontostriatal brain activation from childhood to adulthood during event-related tasks of cognitive control,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 27 (12), 973 –993 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.v27:12 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

77. 

S. Durston et al., “A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development,” Dev. Sci., 9 (1), 1 –8 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1111/DESC.2006.9.issue-1 Google Scholar

78. 

M. H. Johnson et al., “The emergence of the social brain network: evidence from typical and atypical development,” Dev. Psychopathol., 17 (3), 599 –619 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050297 Google Scholar

79. 

A. Senju and M. H. Johnson, “The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development,” Trends Cognit. Sci., 13 (3), 127 –134 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.009 Google Scholar

80. 

A. L. Georgescu et al., “Neural correlates of “social gaze” processing in high-functioning autism under systematic variation of gaze duration,” Neuroimage Clin., 3 (3), 340 –351 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2013.08.014 Google Scholar

81. 

M. K. Belmonte and D. A. Yurgelun-Todd, “Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism,” Cognit. Brain Res., 17 (3), 651 –664 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00189-7 CBRREZ 0926-6410 Google Scholar

82. 

M. Dapretto et al., “Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders,” Nat. Neurosci., 9 (1), 28 –30 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1611 NANEFN 1097-6256 Google Scholar

83. 

M. B. Harms, A. Martin and G. L. Wallace, “Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies,” Neuropsychol. Rev., 20 (3), 290 –322 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-010-9138-6 NERVEJ Google Scholar

84. 

M. D. Kaiser et al., “Neural signatures of autism,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 107 (49), 21223 –21228 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010412107 Google Scholar

85. 

Y.-T. Fan et al., “Empathic arousal and social understanding in individuals with autism: evidence from fMRI and ERP measurements,” Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci., 9 (8), 1203 –1213 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst101 Google Scholar

86. 

M. V. Lombardo et al., “Self-referential cognition and empathy in autism,” PLoS One, 2 (9), e883 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000883 POLNCL 1932-6203 Google Scholar

87. 

D. P. Kennedy, E. Redcay and E. Courchesne, “Failing to deactivate: resting functional abnormalities in autism,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 103 (21), 8275 –8280 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0600674103 Google Scholar

88. 

L. Nummenmaa and A. J. Calder, “Neural mechanisms of social attention,” Trends Cognit. Sci., 13 (3), 135 –143 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.12.006 Google Scholar

89. 

T. Kolodny, C. Mevorach and L. Shalev, “Isolating response inhibition in the brain: parietal versus frontal contribution,” Cortex, 88 173 –185 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.12.012 Google Scholar

90. 

M. Erika-Florence, R. Leech and A. Hampshire, “A functional network perspective on response inhibition and attentional control,” Nat. Commun., 5 4073 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5073 NCAOBW 2041-1723 Google Scholar

91. 

J. Ma et al., “Compensatory brain activation in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder during a simplified Go/No-go task,” J. Neural. Transm., 119 (5), 613 –619 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-011-0744-0 Google Scholar

92. 

L. L. Altshuler et al., “Blunted activation in orbitofrontal cortex during mania: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study,” Biol. Psychiatry, 58 (10), 763 –769 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.012 BIPCBF 0006-3223 Google Scholar

93. 

X. Cui et al., “A quantitative comparison of NIRS and fMRI across multiple cognitive tasks,” Neuroimage, 54 (4), 2808 –2821 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.069 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

94. 

T. Tsujii et al., “Characterization of the acute effects of alcohol on asymmetry of inferior frontal cortex activity during a Go/No-Go task using functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” Psychopharmacology, 217 (4), 595 –603 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2318-0 Google Scholar

95. 

S. E. Christ et al., “Inhibitory control in children with autism spectrum disorder,” J. Autism Dev. Disord., 37 (6), 1155 –1165 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0259-y Google Scholar

Biography

Takahiro Ikeda received his medical degree in Japan in 2007. He was a resident from 2007 to 2012 and has been a research associate since 2012 in the Department of Pediatrics, Jichi Medical University, Tochigi, Japan. Moreover, he was part of the medical staff at the International University of Health and Welfare Rehabilitation Center, Nasu Institute for Developmental Disabilities, Tochigi, Japan, from 2012 to 2014. His research focuses on neuroimaging studies of neurodevelopmental disorders using fNIRS.

Masahiro Hirai received his PhD from the University of Tokyo, Japan, in 2005. He was appointed as an associate professor at the Functional Brain Science Section, Jichi Medical University, Tochigi, Japan. Moreover, he is an international affiliated member of the Centre for Developmental Disorders, Durham University, United Kingdom, since 2017. He has authored 35 peer-reviewed articles, attracting over 500 citations. His research focuses on the neural mechanisms underlying social cognition and cognition in typical and atypical development.

Takeshi Sakurada received his PhD from Tokyo Institute of Technology, Japan, in 2010. He is currently appointed as an assistant professor at the College of Science and Engineering, Ritsumeikan University, Shiga, Japan. He has authored 10 peer-reviewed articles, attracting over 60 citations. His research interests include neurorehabilitation and the study of motor learning.

Yukifumi Monden received his medical degree, in Japan, in 2002 and his PhD from Jichi Medical University, Tochigi, Japan, in 2013. His major professional achievements include his appointment as a clinical associate from 2009 to 2012. He is currently an associate professor in the Department of Pediatrics at Jichi Medical University, and he is also an associate professor at the International University of Health and Welfare, Japan. His research focuses on neuroimaging studies of neurodevelopmental disorders.

Tatsuya Tokuda is a college student at Chuo University, Tokyo, Japan. He has been engaged in fNIRS research since 2015. His research focuses on the clinical application of fNIRS.

Masako Nagashima received her medical degree, in Japan, in 2004 and her PhD from Jichi Medical University, Tochigi, Japan, in 2015. She is an assistant professor in the Department of Pediatrics at Jichi Medical University. She has been on exchange at the University of Tübingen, Germany, since 2017. Her research focuses on neuroimaging studies of neurodevelopmental disorders and the development of a neurofeedback system.

Hideo Shimoizumi received his MD and PhD degrees from Jichi Medical University, Tochigi, Japan, in 1982 and 1992. He is currently appointed as a professor in the School of Health Sciences, and is also the manager at the International University of Health and Welfare Rehabilitation Center, Nasu Institute for Developmental Disabilities, Tochigi, Japan. His research focuses on neurodevelopmental disorders and child abuse.

Ippeita Dan received his PhD from the University of Tokyo Japan, in 2002. He was an associate professor at Jichi Medical University, Tochigi, Japan. He was appointed as a professor at Chuo University, Tokyo, Japan. He has authored 85 peer-reviewed articles, attracting over 5000 citations. His main research interests include the clinical application of fNIRS, methodological development of fNIRS data analyses, and application of psychometrics for marketing in the food industry.

Takanori Yamagata received his medical degree, in Japan, in 1986, and his PhD from Jichi Medical University, Tochigi, Japan, in 1993. He was a research associate in the Department of Molecular and Human Genetics, Baylor College of Medicine, USA, in 1997 to 2000. He has been a professor at the Department of Pediatrics at Jichi Medical University since 2011. His research interests include studies on the molecular pathogenesis of autism spectrum disorder and gene therapy.

CC BY: © The Authors. Published by SPIE under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Unported License. Distribution or reproduction of this work in whole or in part requires full attribution of the original publication, including its DOI.
Takahiro Ikeda, Masahiro Hirai, Takeshi Sakurada, Yukifumi Monden, Tatsuya Tokuda, Masako Nagashima, Hideo Shimoizumi, Ippeita Dan, and Takanori Yamagata "Atypical neural modulation in the right prefrontal cortex during an inhibitory task with eye gaze in autism spectrum disorder as revealed by functional near-infrared spectroscopy," Neurophotonics 5(3), 035008 (5 September 2018). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.5.3.035008
Received: 19 December 2017; Accepted: 2 August 2018; Published: 5 September 2018
Lens.org Logo
CITATIONS
Cited by 10 scholarly publications.
Advertisement
Advertisement
KEYWORDS
Hemodynamics

Modulation

Prefrontal cortex

Brain

Near infrared spectroscopy

Eye

Error analysis

RELATED CONTENT


Back to Top