Open Access
28 May 2019 Prefrontal cortex activation of return-to-work trainees in remission of mental disorders with depressive symptoms compared to that of healthy controls
Hirokazu Atsumori, Akiko Obata, Hiroki Sato, Tsukasa Funane, Ritsuko Yamaguchi, Masashi Kiguchi
Author Affiliations +
Abstract
The increase in the number of patients with mental disorders with depressive symptoms has become a significant problem. To prevent people developing those disorders and help with the effective recovery, it is important to quantitatively and objectively monitor an individual’s mental state. Previous studies have shown the relationship between negative or depressive mood state and human prefrontal cortex (PFC) activation during verbal and spatial working memory tasks based on a near-infrared spectroscopy imaging technique. In this study, we aimed to explore a biomarker of the mental state of people in remission of mental disorders with depressive symptoms using this technique. We obtained the PFC activation of return-to-work (RTW) trainees in remission of those disorders, compared that of healthy controls, and obtained subjective questionnaire scores with the Profile of Mood States. We compared the PFC activation with the questionnaire scores by receiver operating characteristic analysis using a logistic-regression model. The results showed that the PFC activation indicates a healthy state compared to that of the RTW trainees evaluated by area-under-curve analysis. This study demonstrates that our PFC measurement technique will be useful as a quantitative and objective assessment of mental state.

1.

Introduction

The increase in the number of patients with mental disorders with depressive symptoms has become a significant problem. The prevalence rate of depression in high- and low-economic countries ranges from 6.5% to 21%.1 A World Health Organization survey shows that such mental disorders have the highest disability-adjusted life year (a measure of overall disease burden).2 Moreover, depression is a highly recurrent disorder, and the factors associated with recurrence are variable (e.g., onset age, number of depressive episodes, genetic nature, comorbid psychopathology, and vulnerability factor).3,4 Conventional methods using questionnaires have limitations in objectively diagnosing and monitoring mental states.5 One of the solutions to these problems might be to provide patients and/or healthcare professionals with a reliable biomarker to quantify the risks of mental disorder or degrees of recovery. Conventional diagnostic and healthcare/treatment methods for patients have used information from standardized interviewing and/or questionnaire techniques, which gives useful suggestions on patients. However, these techniques might not always give the correct information when a patient’s answers are ambiguous or he/she is confused about his/her feelings. To obtain more accurate information about mental states compared to when using conventional techniques, an objective and quantitative biomarker has been desired in the mental healthcare field.

Functional neuroimaging techniques are a noninvasive tool that would be useful for the study of potential biomarkers related to mental states68 because the identification of the biomarker includes inaccessible brain tissues for study and scarcity of valid animal models.6 A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) technique has been used for investigating the relationship between emotional states induced by a video clip and prefrontal cortex (PFC) activity during working memory (WM) tasks.9 Recently, resting-state fMRI studies have reported that the mental states of depressed patients are related to the functional connectivity in the default mode network area.10 fMRI has a high spatial resolution and monitors activation in deep areas of brain tissues but has a constraint on subjects. However, the near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) technique11,12 noninvasively monitors cerebral hemodynamic changes when the subject is in an ordinary posture, such as sitting, and has been used for research and clinical purposes,1316 and a differential diagnostic method for mental disorders based on the NIRS technique has been proposed. This method was approved as an advanced medical technology by the Japan Ministry of Health, Labor, and Welfare (MHLW) in 20097 and then was covered by the insurance of the MHLW in 2014.7,17 This method gives diagnostic support information for differentiation of major psychiatric disorders with depressive symptoms by analyzing human frontotemporal NIRS signals.7

We assumed that the usefulness of NIRS in mental healthcare could be expanded from clinical situations to daily use situations, such as monitoring an individual’s mood states in the workplace. Previous studies have suggested a significant correlation between an individual’s natural mood states and cognitive functions using an NIRS system and revealed that subjects reporting a higher level of depressive mood or negative mood (the total score for tension, depression, anxiety, fatigue, and confusion) showed a lower level of PFC activity during verbal WM tasks without a mood-induction method.1822 These studies have revealed the mood–cognition interaction in the PFC, which has been considered to play a crucial role in cognitive functions,2325 and have shown that the PFC activation could be a biomarker as an indication of the natural mood state for healthy individuals. However, these studies have focused on group-analysis results while a robust biomarker also requires a high accuracy at the individual level. Thus, it is unclear whether an objective biomarker, such as an NIRS measurement, has an advantage over a subjective mood scale, such as a questionnaire. Thus, it is necessary to compare the NIRS measurement with a subjective “logistic-regression model” mood scale for considering its advantages.

In this study, we aimed to explore the advantages of NIRS by comparing two kinds of indication: the NIRS measurement indication and the questionnaire-based indication with the Profile of Mood States (POMS).26,27 The NIRS measurement and questionnaire-based indications were used to discriminate between subjects in remission from mental disorders and healthy controls using a logistic-regression model. Then, we compared those indications by receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis28 based on the hypothesis of determining advantages for an NIRS-based biomarker over a questionnaire-based indication.

2.

Methods

2.1.

Participants

Twenty-nine healthy Japanese volunteers (12 females and 17 males, mean age=35.9±7.7) and twenty-one return-to-work (RTW) trainees in remission of mental disorders with depressive symptoms (5 females and 16 males, mean age=40.9±6.31) participated in this study. The RTW trainees have attended this trial instead of attending some kind of normal training class for returning to work, which is given by clinical psychologists in an RTW facility. The facility obtains statements about each trainee’s mental states (e.g., depressive symptoms) and the will to return to work but does not obtain other private information such as education level and treatment, which are managed by the doctors in charge who are in different hospitals/clinics. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after they were informed of the purpose, procedures, risks, benefits, and voluntary nature of the experiments. This study was conducted according to the regulations of the internal review board of the Central Research Laboratory, Hitachi, Ltd.

2.2.

Mood State Measurement

The participants’ mood state was assessed using the Japanese edition of a short form of the POMS.26,27 The participants rated their mood using 30 mood-related adjectives (angry, sad, vital, etc.) on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (“not at all”) to 4 (“extremely”). This rating enabled us to estimate six mood-related measures: tension-anxiety (T-A), depression-dejection (D), anger-hostility (A-H), vigor (V), fatigue (F), and confusion (C).

2.3.

Prefrontal Cortex Activity Measurement

We used two types of multichannel functional NIRS system for measuring the changes in the cerebral blood concentration of the participants.11,12,29,30 One was a commercial model of an NIRS system (ETG-4000, Hitachi Medical Corp.),11,12 and the other was a prototype of a wearable optical topography (WOT) system developed by the Central Research Laboratory, Hitachi, Ltd.29,30 The former was used to measure healthy controls in an experimental room, and the latter was for used for participants in the RTW program because of its portability (we could use it at the other site where the program was operated). Each NIRS device emits near-infrared light (2 mW) at two wavelengths onto the scalp and detects the reflected light. The NIRS method measures changes in the product of hemoglobin (Hb) concentration and effective optical path length in human brain tissue. The unit of Hb change is molar concentration (mM) multiplied by optical path length (mm). The changes in oxygenated Hb (HbO2) and deoxygenated Hb (HHb) signals are estimated using absorption coefficient spectra for the two wavelengths. Both systems have the same source–detector distance (3 cm) and the same laser power (2 mW). The differences of both systems were the sampling rate (the former was 10 Hz, and the latter was 5 Hz) and the pair of wavelengths (the former used 695 and 830 nm, and the latter used 754 and 830 nm). However, the waveforms of HbO2 and HHb measured with the latter prototype were confirmed as almost the same as those of the commercial model.29,30 The ETG-4000 and WOT systems cover the entire forehead, and the measurement positions of each system were estimated by a probabilistic method used in previous studies.3133 The channel positions on the prefrontal area of both systems are shown in Fig. 1; however, the positions were not completely the same, so we chose the five channel positions numbered 1 to 5 that are closest to each other in the PFC for analysis.

Fig. 1

Channel positions of NIRS systems. Lilac and red circles indicate positions of ETG-4000 and WOT systems on cerebral cortex, respectively. Each channel position is defined as center point of each light source and detector. Five channels numbered 1 to 5 and surrounded by dashed line in prefrontal area were used for analysis.

JBO_24_5_056008_f001.png

2.4.

Working Memory Tasks

We used verbal and spatial delayed match-to-sample WM tasks to measure the PFC activities [Fig. 2(a)]. These were the same as those used in previous NIRS studies.1822 Each task trial started with a 1.5-s presentation of the target stimuli (target) on a PC screen [Fig. 2(b)]. After presenting a delay (black screen with white fixation cross) for the following 7 s, a test stimulus for retrieval (probe) was then presented for 2 s or until the participant responded. The participant responded by pressing a button on a handheld game pad connected to the PC, and the test stimulus disappeared when the button was pressed within 2 s. The intervals between the trial finish and the target onset in the next trial were randomized from 16 to 22 s for avoiding participant habituation or prediction of the target onset. For a verbal WM task, a set of four Japanese hiragana characters was presented as the target, and a Japanese katakana character was presented as the probe. The participants were asked to judge whether the probe character corresponded to one of the four target characters. For the judgment, they used their right index and middle fingers to press the “yes” and “no” button, respectively. Because the characters presented as target and probe were in different Japanese morphograms, the participants made their decisions on the basis of the phonetic information conveyed by the characters, not their form. Auditory cues of 1000- and 800-Hz pure tones of 100-ms duration were presented at the target and probe onsets, respectively. For a spatial WM task, the target was given by the locations of four squares randomly arranged at eight peripheral locations. The probe was given by presenting a square at one of the eight locations. The participant was asked to judge whether the probe square location corresponded to one of the target locations.

Fig. 2

(a) Spatial and verbal WM tasks and (b) time sequence of a trial.

JBO_24_5_056008_f002.png

2.5.

Experimental Procedure

The participants were seated in a comfortable chair in a quiet room. Their mood states were assessed with POMS26,27 at the beginning of the experiment. Next, the participants received computer-automated instructions that were followed by a brief practice session to familiarize the participants with the tasks. The practice session included two to five trials of each task depending on the participants’ familiarization with the tasks. Thereafter, NIRS measurements were conducted while the participants performed the WM tasks. The tasks were organized into two sessions, one for the verbal WM task and the other for the spatial WM task, with a counterbalanced order across participants. Each session included five trials of either one of the tasks, and the sessions were separated by a short break (1  min). The duration of the NIRS measurements was 15  min, and the whole experiment took about 30 min.

2.6.

Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Data Analysis

For the NIRS data analysis, we used MATLAB (The MathWorks, Inc.) and the plug-in-based software we developed. We defined a block for each signal as the period from 1 s before presenting the target (which lasts 1.5 s followed by a 7-s delay for memorization) to 16 s after starting presenting the test, 25.5 s in total. We conducted five blocks of spatial WM tasks and five blocks of verbal WM tasks. To remove the components originating from slow fluctuations and the high-frequency noise of blood flow, a 1.5-Hz low-pass filter and 0.02-Hz high-pass filter were applied to the signals. Then, the HbO2 signal in each block was baseline corrected by subtracting the average value of the first 1 s. Finally, we defined the PFC activity period as the period from 5 s after presenting the target to just before starting presenting the test (3.5 s).16 We focused on the HbO2 signal in the statistical analysis as the previous studies found main effects in this signal.1820 We used the t-statistic value of HbO2 signals during the PFC activity period from the five blocks for spatial and verbal WM tasks and denote them as HbO2spat and HbO2verb, respectively, as the measures estimating the PFC activity for each channel.

2.7.

Modeling Indication of Healthy Control/Return-to-Work Trainee

To estimate the indication of the difference between healthy controls and RTW trainees, we applied multivariable logistic-regression analysis to the POMS scores, HbO2spat, and HbO2verb (Ch 1 to 5, see Fig. 1). In this analysis, each participant’s data were labeled with a binary value, which is 1 if the participant is a healthy control and 0 if the participant is an RTW trainee. Next, manual backward selection was used to eliminate nonsignificant components from each logistic-regression model and repeat the regression analysis using only significant component(s) (p<0.05).

To evaluate the performance of logistic-regression models from the three sets of POMS, HbO2spat, and HbO2verb, we used ROC analysis.26 The area-under-curve (AUC) was used for quantitative comparison of the ROC curve obtained from those logistic-regression models, and its 95% confidence interval (CI) was calculated by bootstrap estimation with 10,000 replications.

3.

Results

3.1.

Time Courses of HbO2 and HHb Signals

Figure 3 shows the mean time courses of HbO2 and HHb signals of two channels (Ch 1 and 5) in response to spatial and verbal WM tasks for the healthy control and RTW trainee groups as typical waveforms, where PFC activation of these channels (Ch 1 and 5) are statistically significant (p<0.05) for the HbO2verb of the healthy control, as shown in Table 1. In Fig. 3, the horizontal and vertical axes indicate the time in which the zero value means the target onset and the HbO2 and HHb signals, respectively. Changes in the HbO2 signals during spatial and verbal WM memory tasks for both groups increase after the target onset and decrease around the baseline in the several seconds after the probe stimuli, and those in the HHb signals are almost flat compared to those in the HbO2 signals. However, the amplitudes in the first peak (around 7 to 9 s) after the target onset for the RTW trainees are less than half of those for the healthy controls.

Fig. 3

Mean time courses of HbO2 and HHb signals of two representative channels (Ch 1 and 5) to spatial and verbal WM memory tasks for (a) healthy controls and (b) RTW trainees.

JBO_24_5_056008_f003.png

Table 1

The t-statistic value of PFC activity (HbO2) of all participants.

ChannelHealthy controlRTW trainee
SpatialVerbalSpatialVerbal
13.7 (p<0.01)3.7 (p<0.01)2.0 (p<0.1)1.3 (p>0.1)
22.6 (p<0.05)2.0 (p<0.1)0.83 (p>0.1)0.52 (p>0.1)
32.2 (p<0.05)1.8 (p<0.1)0.15 (p>0.1)0.21 (p>0.1)
42.6 (p<0.05)2.0 (p<0.1)0.13 (p>0.1)0.62 (p>0.1)
51.6 (p>0.1)2.6 (p<0.05)0.51 (p>0.1)0.29 (p>0.1)

3.2.

Logistic-Regression Models of Prefrontal Cortex Activity and Profile of Mood States

The logistic-regression results from HbO2spat and HbO2verb (calculated from HbO2 signals) are summarized in Table 2. This table shows the regression coefficients and p values of the target channels. No significant components are observed in the results from HbO2spat. The statistically significant components are Ch 1 and 3 of HbO2verb. We applied logistic-regression analysis to those two channels again since we assumed that the other channels contribute less to discriminate the RTW trainee group and healthy control group than those two channels (Ch 1 and 3), and then we obtained significant results (Ch 1: regression coef=0.19, p=0.0065, Ch 3: 0.70, p=0.035).

Table 2

Logistic-regression results for HbO2 data.

ChannelHbO2spatHbO2verb
Regression coefficientp valueRegression coefficientp value
10.360.240.800.049
20.210.660.210.56
30.580.1410.026
40.720.170.450.23
50.350.170.160.74
Const.0.120.760.0520.90

The logistic-regression results from POMS are shown in Table 3. The statistically significant component is the vigor score (POMS_V). We applied logistic-regression analysis to only POMS_V, and then we obtained significant results (POMS_V: regression coef=0.29, p=0.0062).

Table 3

Logistic-regression results for POMS™ data.

Regression coefficientp value
T-A0.0720.50
D0.00800.95
A-H0.180.15
V0.310.021
F0.0580.60
C0.0160.92
Const.1.40.31

3.3.

Receiver Operating Characteristic Analysis

To assess the performance of discrimination between healthy controls and RTW trainees based on logistic-regression models of HbO2verb and POMS_V, we used ROC curve analysis. One ROC curve shows the relationship of sensitivity versus 1 — specificity when the criterion to discriminate the binary state based on the designed model is changed; thus, the model with higher sensitivity and higher specificity is closer to the upper left corner. Figure 4 shows the ROC plots derived from HbO2verb and POMS_V. The solid diagonal line from (0, 0) to (1, 1) indicates the no-discrimination line (chance level), and its AUC is equal to 0.5. In Fig. 3, the ROC curves of both HbO2verb and POMS_V fall between the upper left corner and the no-discrimination line. The AUC of HbO2verb is 0.792 (95% CI 0.651 to 0.918) and that of POMS_V is 0.755 (95% CI 0.591 to 0.891), indicating moderate power for discrimination, but the AUC of HbO2verb is slightly higher than that of POMS_V. To estimate the best discrimination point of each AUC curve, the point nearest to the upper left point was selected and marked with a circle on the curve. At the best discrimination point of HbO2verb, the sensitivity and specificity are 75.9% and 81%, respectively. At that of POMS_V, those values are 90% and 57.1%, respectively.

Fig. 4

ROC curves.

JBO_24_5_056008_f004.png

4.

Discussion

In this study, we aimed to explore an effective biomarker of an individual’s mental state and evaluated the PFC activation of RTW trainees in remission of mental disorders and that of healthy controls. We applied the logistic-regression model to the PFC activation of the participants and found that the PFC activation in the right and central frontopolar cortex during a verbal WM task are statistically significant to discriminate those participants into two groups: the RTW trainee group and the healthy control group. This result is consistent with some previous articles that have shown that PFC activities during a verbal WM task are inversely correlated with a negative/depressive mood state compared to those during a spatial WM task1821 and might be supported by a previous fMRI study that reported that an induced negative mood-attenuated PFC activity during a verbal WM task.34 In our study, statistically significant coefficients in the logistic-regression model of the right and central area (Ch 1 and 3, respectively) in frontopolar cortex are positive (0.19, p=0.0065) and negative (0.70, p=0.035), respectively. The model outputs a value of 0 and 1, which indicates that the higher value means a more healthy state because “1” means healthy and “0” does not, so the NIRS data at the right area in frontopolar cortex correspond to the results in previous studies.1821 Previous studies have shown that frontopolar cortex plays a crucial role in WM, attentional resource allocation (referred as branching), and planning.35,36 Thus, our result suggests that the difference of these functions between the RTW trainees and the healthy controls is reflected. However, we need to investigate the activation of the central area in frontopolar cortex because it is not clear how this activation contributes to the model and it has the opposite effects of activation from Ch 1 (positive) and Ch 3 (negative).

On the basis of the POMS scores, we also made a logistic-regression model and obtained the statistically significant component from only the POMS vigor score (POMS_V), which indicates that the regression coefficient is 0.29 (p=0.0062). This result shows that the higher POMS_V score means a more vigorous state.

Based on the above results, we compared the logistic-regression models derived from PFC activities and POMS_V using ROC analysis. In this analysis, the ROC curves of both PFC activity and POMS_V are over the chance level. The AUC of PFC activities is 0.792 (95% CI 0.625 to 0.898), which indicates moderate power to discriminate a healthy control and an RTW trainee. The AUC of POMS_V is 0.755 (95% CI 0.625 to 0.875), which also indicates moderate power. The 95% CIs of those two AUCs overlap well, but that of PFC activities is slightly higher than that of POMS_V.

In the ROC analysis, we also tried to extract the point nearest to the upper left point (0, 1) on each ROC curve, which gives one of the best discrimination points. At the nearest point on the ROC curve of PFC activities, the sensitivity and specificity are 75.9% and 81%, respectively, and for POMS_V, those values are 90% and 57.1%, respectively. The results show that the sensitivity of POMS_V is very high but its specificity is about the chance level, which indicates that it is difficult to discriminate the healthy controls and the RTW trainees using only the vigor score of POMS. The sensitivity of PFC activities (75.9%) is less than that of POMS_V (90%) but the specificity is high (81%), so the measures of PFC activities have the potential to discriminate the healthy controls and the RTW trainees. However, we have not obtained enough results showing advantages of the PFC activation over the POMS_V to discriminate two populations (RTW trainee group and healthy control group) in our study because the AUC of PFC activities and that of POMS_V indicate moderate power (0.792 and 0.755, respectively), and those 95% CIs overlap (0.625 to 0.898 and 0.625 to 0.875, respectively); thus, we need more investigation with a larger sample size and more participants’ details.

Our study has several limitations. We used two kinds of NIRS measurement systems: a floor-standing and commercial model using optical fibers (ETG-4000, Hitachi Medical Corp.) for healthy controls and a prototype of the WOT system that we previously developed,29,30 because the former system was difficult to install on the RTW facility where there was not enough space. The performance and specification of the latter have been confirmed by our previous studies, but the measurement positions were limited and slightly different from those of the former model. Thus, we confirmed the measurement positions of each system using the spatial registration method.3133 We need to analyze NIRS data from wide range of the PFC by obtaining the data from the same measurement positions with the same system for different groups for further investigation.

Another limitation is that we did not control medical information for analysis since we have not obtained such information about the RTW trainees (diagnosis, medical history, treatment in hospital/clinic, education level, and so on) except for depressive symptoms, age, and gender, because we had difficulties to obtain such information from doctors of the trainees from different hospitals/clinics. In addition, we did not control the gender of the RTW trainees (5 females and 16 males) for analysis, so it is difficult to discuss the gender differences in this study. Thus, this study was limited to evaluation of the difference of depressive symptoms between the RTW trainees and the healthy controls. We need to conduct precise analyses for various aspects of mental disorders with depressive symptoms by accumulating biometric measures and medical information.

The other limitation is that it is difficult to distinguish whether the indication derived from the PFC activities in our study shows the participant’s state or trait because we have not obtained participants’ personality trait scores that were evaluated in the previous study.19 We need to investigate more to clarify whether the indication shows the state or trait.

Finally, the results of this study show the potential to give one of the indications from the PFC activation for discrimination of RTW trainees in remission of mental disorders with depressive symptoms and healthy controls. By minimizing the above limitations, we hope that the current results can lead to more practical applications, such as individual monitoring for both healthy and RTW trainees.

Disclosures

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the participants for taking part in our research by wearing an NIRS system and answering the POMS questionnaire for data collection. We are grateful to Ryuta Aoki, Kisou Kubota, Hideaki Koizumi, Teruhiko Higuchi, and Souichiro Nomura for our helpful discussions.

References

1. 

R. C. Kessler and E. J. Bromet, “The epidemiology of depression across cultures,” Annu. Rev. Publ. Health, 34 119 –138 (2013). http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031912-114409 Google Scholar

2. 

, “The global burden of disease 2004 update,” (2004) http://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/2004_report_update/en/ April 2017). Google Scholar

3. 

S. L. Burcusa and W. G. Iacono, “Risk for recurrence in depression,” Clin. Psychol. Rev., 27 (8), 959 –985 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.02.005 Google Scholar

4. 

K. Suija et al., “Factors associated with recurrent depression: a prospective study in family practice,” Family Pract., 28 22 –28 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/fampra/cmq076 Google Scholar

5. 

A. J. Mitchell et al., “Do primary care physicians have particular difficulty identifying late-life depression? A meta-analysis stratified by age,” Psychother. Psychosom., 79 (5), 285 –294 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000318295 Google Scholar

6. 

B. M. Hager and M. S. Keshavan, “Neuroimaging biomarkers for psychosis,” Curr. Behav. Neurosci. Rep., 2 102 –111 (2015). http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40473-015-0035-4 Google Scholar

7. 

R. Takizawa et al., “Neuroimaging-aided differential diagnosis of the depressive state,” NeuroImage, 85 (1), 498 –507 (2014). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.126 Google Scholar

8. 

C. H. Y. Fu and S. G. Costafreda, “Neuroimaging-based biomarkers in psychiatry: clinical opportunities of a paradigm shift,” Can. J. Psychiatry, 58 (9), 499 –508 (2013). http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/070674371305800904 Google Scholar

9. 

J. R. Gray, T. S. Braver and M. E. Raichle, “Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex,” 4115 –4120 (2002). Google Scholar

10. 

S. Yokoyama et al., “Effects of behavioral activation on default mode network connectivity in subthreshold depression: a preliminary resting-state fMRI study,” J. Affective Disord., 227 156 –163 (2018). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.10.021 Google Scholar

11. 

A. Maki et al., “Spatial and temporal analysis of human motor activity using noninvasive NIR topography,” Med. Phys., 22 (12), 1997 –2005 (1995). http://dx.doi.org/10.1118/1.597496 MPHYA6 0094-2405 Google Scholar

12. 

H. Koizumi et al., “Non-invasive brain-function imaging by optical topography,” TrAC Trends Anal. Chem., 24 (2), 147 –156 (2005). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.trac.2004.11.002 Google Scholar

13. 

A. C. Ehlis et al., “Application of functional near-infrared spectroscopy in psychiatry,” NeuroImage, 85 478 –488 (2014). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.067 Google Scholar

14. 

M. Peña et al., “Sounds and silence: an optical topography study of language recognition at birth,” 11702 –11705 (2003). Google Scholar

15. 

G. Taga et al., “Brain imaging in awake infants by near-infrared optical topography,” 10722 –10727 (2003). Google Scholar

16. 

T. Funane et al., “Synchronous activity of two people’s prefrontal cortices during a cooperative task measured by simultaneous near-infrared spectroscopy,” J. Biomed. Opt., 16 (7), 077011 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.3602853 Google Scholar

17. 

, “Outline of FY 2014 revision of medical fee,” (2014) http://www.mhlw.go.jp/stf/seisakunitsuite/bunya/0000032996.html April 2017). Google Scholar

18. 

R. Aoki et al., “Relationship of negative mood with prefrontal cortex activity during working memory tasks: an optical topography study,” Neurosci. Res., 70 (2), 189 –196 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2011.02.011 Google Scholar

19. 

R. Aoki et al., “Correlation between prefrontal cortex activity during working memory tasks and natural mood independent of personality effects: an optical topography study,” Psychiatry Res., 212 (1), 79 –87 (2013). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.10.009 Google Scholar

20. 

H. Sato et al., “Correlation of within-individual fluctuation of depressed mood with prefrontal cortex activity during verbal working memory task: optical topography study,” J. Biomed. Opt., 16 (12), 126007 (2011). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.3662448 Google Scholar

21. 

H. Sato et al., “Replication of the correlation between natural mood states and working memory-related prefrontal activity measured by near-infrared spectroscopy in a German sample,” Front. Hum. Neurosci., 8 37 (2014). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00037 Google Scholar

22. 

J. Watanabe, H. Atsumori and M. Kiguchi, “Informal face-to-face interaction improves mood state reflected in prefrontal cortex activity,” Front. Hum. Neurosci., 10 194 (2016). Google Scholar

23. 

M. D'Esposito, “From cognitive to neural models of working memory,” Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 362 761 –772 (2007). http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2086 Google Scholar

24. 

M. D'Esposito et al., “The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory,” Nature, 378 279 –281 (1995). http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/378279a0 Google Scholar

25. 

M. D'Esposito, B. R. Postle and B. Rypma, “Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies,” Exp. Brain Res., 133 3 –11 (2000). http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002210000395 Google Scholar

26. 

P. M. McNair, M. Lorr and L. F. Droppleman, Profile of Mood States Manual, Educational and Industrial Testing Service, San Diego, California (1971). Google Scholar

27. 

K. Yokoyama et al., “Production of the Japanese edition of profile of mood states (POMS): assessment of reliability and validity,” Nihon Koshu Eisei Zasshi, 37 (11), 913 –918 (1990). Google Scholar

28. 

M. H. Zweig and G. Campbell, “Receiver-operating characteristic (ROC) plots: a fundamental evaluation tool in clinical medicine,” Clin. Chem., 39 (4), 561 –577 (1993). Google Scholar

29. 

H. Atsumori et al., “Development of wearable optical topography system for mapping the prefrontal cortex activation,” Rev. Sci. Instrum., 80 (4), 043704 (2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.1063/1.3115207 Google Scholar

30. 

H. Atsumori et al., “Noninvasive imaging of prefrontal activation during attention-demanding tasks performed while walking using a wearable optical topography system,” J. Biomed. Opt., 15 (4), 046002 (2010). http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/1.3462996 Google Scholar

31. 

A. K. Singh et al., “Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI,” NeuroImage, 27 (4), 842 –851 (2005). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.019 Google Scholar

32. 

M. Okamoto and I. Dan, “Automated cortical projection of head-surface locations for transcranial functional brain mapping,” NeuroImage, 26 (1), 18 –28 (2005). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.018 Google Scholar

33. 

M. Okamoto et al., “Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10-20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping,” NeuroImage, 21 (1), 99 –111 (2004). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.026 Google Scholar

34. 

S. Qin et al., “Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,” Biol. Psychiatry, 66 (1), 25 –32 (2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.006 Google Scholar

35. 

E. Koechlin et al., “The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition,” Nature, 399 148 –151 (1999). http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/20178 Google Scholar

36. 

E. Koechlin et al., “Dissociating the role of the medial and lateral anterior prefrontal cortex in human planning,” 7651 –7656 (2000). Google Scholar

Biography

Hirokazu Atsumori is a researcher in the Center for Exploratory Research, R&D Group, Hitachi, Ltd., Japan. He has been working on the research and development of optical topography, a functional neuroimaging technique based on near-infrared spectroscopy, since 2002. Currently, he is engaged in the development of a wearable and compact optical topography system for monitoring prefrontal cortex activities and its application to new research fields.

Biographies of the other authors are not available.

CC BY: © The Authors. Published by SPIE under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Unported License. Distribution or reproduction of this work in whole or in part requires full attribution of the original publication, including its DOI.
Hirokazu Atsumori, Akiko Obata, Hiroki Sato, Tsukasa Funane, Ritsuko Yamaguchi, and Masashi Kiguchi "Prefrontal cortex activation of return-to-work trainees in remission of mental disorders with depressive symptoms compared to that of healthy controls," Journal of Biomedical Optics 24(5), 056008 (28 May 2019). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.JBO.24.5.056008
Received: 16 May 2018; Accepted: 6 May 2019; Published: 28 May 2019
Lens.org Logo
CITATIONS
Cited by 2 scholarly publications.
Advertisement
Advertisement
KEYWORDS
Control systems

Near infrared spectroscopy

Mental disorders

Prefrontal cortex

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

Statistical analysis

Statistical modeling

RELATED CONTENT


Back to Top